Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

The striatum multiplexes contextual and kinematic information to constrain motor habits execution

Subjects

Abstract

The striatum is required for the acquisition of procedural memories, but its contribution to motor control once learning has occurred is unclear. We created a task in which rats learned a difficult motor sequence characterized by fine-tuned changes in running speed adjusted to spatial and temporal constraints. After training and extensive practice, we found that the behavior was habitual, yet tetrode recordings in the dorsolateral striatum (DLS) revealed continuous integrative representations of running speed, position and time. These representations were weak in naive rats that were hand-guided to perform the same sequence and developed slowly after learning. Finally, DLS inactivation in well-trained animals preserved the structure of the sequence while increasing its trial-by-trial variability. We conclude that, after learning, the DLS continuously integrates task-relevant information to constrain the execution of motor habits. Our results provide a straightforward mechanism by which the basal ganglia may contribute to habit formation and motor control.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: A fixed interval estimation task adapted to treadmill locomotion favored the learning of a stereotyped running sequence.
Figure 2: Evidence for habitual performance of the running sequence.
Figure 3: Continuous sequential modulation of striatal spiking activity during sequence execution.
Figure 4: Linear and integrative representation of running speed, position and time after learning.
Figure 5: Speed-related spiking activity changes more slowly than running speed.
Figure 6: Position and running speed representations in the DLS are specific of motor habits.
Figure 7: Inactivation of the DLS increases trial-by-trial variability of running sequence execution.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Woodworth, R.S. The Accuracy of Voluntary Movement. Psychol. Rev. III, 1–120 (1899).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Lee, T.D. Motor Control in Everyday Actions (Human Kinetics, 2011).

  3. Bailey, K.R. & Mair, R.G. The role of striatum in initiation and execution of learned action sequences in rats. J. Neurosci. 26, 1016–1025 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Barnes, T.D., Kubota, Y., Hu, D., Jin, D.Z. & Graybiel, A.M. Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature 437, 1158–1161 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Costa, R.M., Cohen, D. & Nicolelis, M.A.L. Differential corticostriatal plasticity during fast and slow motor skill learning in mice. Curr. Biol. 14, 1124–1134 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Jin, X. & Costa, R.M. Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature 466, 457–462 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Jog, M.S., Kubota, Y., Connolly, C.I., Hillegaart, V. & Graybiel, A.M. Building neural representations of habits. Science 286, 1745–1749 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Packard, M.G. & McGaugh, J.L. Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 65, 65–72 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Yin, H.H. et al. Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 333–341 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Tang, C., Pawlak, A.P., Prokopenko, V. & West, M.O. Changes in activity of the striatum during formation of a motor habit. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 1212–1227 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Deffains, M., Legallet, E. & Apicella, P. Modulation of neuronal activity in the monkey putamen associated with changes in the habitual order of sequential movements. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1355–1369 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Desmurget, M. & Turner, R.S. Motor sequences and the basal ganglia: kinematics, not habits. J. Neurosci. 30, 7685–7690 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Miyachi, S., Hikosaka, O., Miyashita, K., Kárádi, Z. & Rand, M.K. Differential roles of monkey striatum in learning of sequential hand movement. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 1–5 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Miyachi, S., Hikosaka, O. & Lu, X. Differential activation of monkey striatal neurons in the early and late stages of procedural learning. Exp. Brain Res. 146, 122–126 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Lehéricy, S. et al. Distinct basal ganglia territories are engaged in early and advanced motor sequence learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 12566–12571 (2005).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Wymbs, N.F., Bassett, D.S., Mucha, P.J., Porter, M.A. & Grafton, S.T. Differential recruitment of the sensorimotor putamen and frontoparietal cortex during motor chunking in humans. Neuron 74, 936–946 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Graybiel, A.M. Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 359–387 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Shmuelof, L. & Krakauer, J.W. Are we ready for a natural history of motor learning? Neuron 72, 469–476 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Turner, R.S. & Desmurget, M. Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 704–716 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Gerfen, C.R. & Bolam, J.P. The neuroanatomical organization of the basal ganglia. in Handbook of Basal Ganglia Structure and Function (eds. Steiner, H. & Tseng, K.) 3–28 (Academic Press, 2010).

  21. Winn, P., Wilson, D.I.G. & Redgrave, P. Subcortical connections of the basal ganglia. in Handbook of Basal Ganglia Structure and Function (eds. Steiner, H. & Tseng, K.) 397–408 (Academic Press, 2010).

  22. Shadmehr, R. & Krakauer, J.W. A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 359–381 (2008).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Yin, H.H. & Knowlton, B.J. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 464–476 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Balleine, B.W. & Dickinson, A. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology 37, 407–419 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Apicella, P., Scarnati, E., Ljungberg, T. & Schultz, W. Neuronal activity in monkey striatum related to the expectation of predictable environmental events. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 945–960 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Apicella, P., Ljungberg, T., Scarnati, E. & Schultz, W. Responses to reward in monkey dorsal and ventral striatum. Exp. Brain Res. 85, 491–500 (1991).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. West, M.O. et al. A region in the dorsolateral striatum of the rat exhibiting single-unit correlations with specific locomotor limb movements. J. Neurophysiol. 64, 1233–1246 (1990).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Shi, L.H., Luo, F., Woodward, D.J. & Chang, J.Y. Neural responses in multiple basal ganglia regions during spontaneous and treadmill locomotion tasks in rats. Exp. Brain Res. 157, 303–314 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Whitlock, J.R., Pfuhl, G., Dagslott, N., Moser, M.-B. & Moser, E.I. Functional split between parietal and entorhinal cortices in the rat. Neuron 73, 789–802 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cho, J. & West, M.O. Distributions of single neurons related to body parts in the lateral striatum of the rat. Brain Res. 756, 241–246 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gage, G.J., Stoetzner, C.R., Wiltschko, A.B. & Berke, J.D. Selective activation of striatal fast-spiking interneurons during choice execution. Neuron 67, 466–479 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Freeze, B.S., Kravitz, V., Hammack, N., Berke, J.D. & Kreitzer, C. Control of Basal Ganglia Output by Direct and Indirect Pathway Projection Neurons. J. Neurosci. 33, 18531–18539 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Kravitz, A.V. et al. Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry. Nature 466, 622–626 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. McGeorge, A.J. & Faull, R.L.M. The organization of the projection from the cerebral cortex to the striatum in the rat. Neuroscience 29, 503–537 (1989).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Ranade, S., Hangya, B. & Kepecs, A. Multiple modes of phase locking between sniffing and whisking during active exploration. J. Neurosci. 33, 8250–8256 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Ledberg, A. & Robbe, D. Locomotion-related oscillatory body movements at 6–12 Hz modulate the hippocampal theta rhythm. PLoS ONE 6, e27575 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Berke, J.D., Breck, J.T. & Eichenbaum, H. Striatal versus hippocampal representations during win-stay maze performance. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 1575–1587 (2009).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. van der Meer, M.A., Johnson, A., Schmitzer-Torbert, N.C. & Redish, A.D. Report triple dissociation of information processing in dorsal striatum, ventral striatum, and hippocampus on a learned spatial decision task. Neuron 67, 25–32 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Yamin, H.G., Stern, E.A. & Cohen, D. Parallel processing of environmental recognition and locomotion in the mouse striatum. J. Neurosci. 33, 473–484 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Fee, M.S. Oculomotor learning revisited: a model of reinforcement learning in the basal ganglia incorporating an efference copy of motor actions. Front. Neural Circuits 6, 38 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Mazzoni, P., Hristova, A. & Krakauer, J.W. Why don't we move faster? Parkinson's disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J. Neurosci. 27, 7105–7116 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Wang, A.Y., Miura, K. & Uchida, N. The dorsomedial striatum encodes net expected return, critical for energizing performance vigor. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 639–647 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Jin, X., Tecuapetla, F. & Costa, R.M. Basal ganglia subcircuits distinctively encode the parsing and concatenation of action sequences. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 423–430 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Schmidt, R., Leventhal, D.K., Mallet, N., Chen, F. & Berke, J.D. Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1118–1124 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Cui, G. et al. Concurrent activation of striatal direct and indirect pathways during action initiation. Nature 494, 238–242 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Ding, L. & Gold, J.I. The basal ganglia's contributions to perceptual decision making. Neuron 79, 640–649 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Ferguson, J.E., Boldt, C. & Redish, A.D. Creating low-impedance tetrodes by electroplating with additives. Sens. Actuators A Phys. 156, 388–393 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Harris, K.D., Henze, D.A., Csicsvari, J., Hirase, H. & Buzsáki, G. Accuracy of tetrode spike separation as determined by simultaneous intracellular and extracellular measurements. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 401–414 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Hazan, L., Zugaro, M. & Buzsáki, G. Klusters, NeuroScope, NDManager: a free software suite for neurophysiological data processing and visualization. J. Neurosci. Methods 155, 207–216 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Amarasingham, A., Harrison, M.T., Hatsopoulos, N.G. & Geman, S. Conditional modeling and the jitter method of spike resampling. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 517–531 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank J. Krakauer, K. Diba, J. Epsztein, O. Manzoni and H. Martin for excellent discussions and critical reading of this and/or earlier version of the manuscript, I. Cordon Morillas, C. Sales-Carbonell and L. Lalla for help with training of the animals, J. Perez-Ortega for help with LabView Vision, A. Brovelli for advice with multiple regression analysis, and R. Martinez for technical help. This work was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (Ramon-Y-Cajal program, D.R.) EU-fp7 (International Reintegration Grant, IRG230976, D.R.; Marie Curie International Incoming Fellowship, IIF253873, P.E.R.-O.), INSERM (Avenir Program, D.R.), European Research Council (ERC-2013-CoG – 615699_NeuroKinematics, D.R.) and the Mexican Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (P.E.R.-O.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

P.E.R.-O. performed the experiments. D.R. and P.E.R.-O. designed the experiments, analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Robbe.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Additional illustrations of the front-rear-front running sequence in several rats.

(a–c) Trajectories of 6 animals during single sessions after extensive training (at least 2 months of daily training). The goal time was fixed at 5 s (a, Rat 4 and 9), 6 s (b, Rat 6) or 7 s (c, Rat 2, 7, 17). Trajectories of Rat 2, 2 months after shortening the treadmill, are shown in the bottom panel in c.

Supplementary Figure 2 High level of stereotypy in trained animals.

(a) Trajectory of the left forelimb of a well-trained animal during a single correct trial. The trough of each oscillation (yellow dots) corresponds to the time of the step cycle at which the left forelimb is stretched forward. The last step of the sequence is indicated in red. (b) Forelimb stretched times during a single session. Trials are aligned relative to the last step of the running sequence (red arrow) and sorted according to the time of the last but one step. (c) Filtered trajectories (same session than b), realigned to the end of the sequence (left) and speed profiles (right) derived from the trajectories. Dashed red lined indicates treadmill's speed.

Supplementary Figure 3 Electrophysiological procedures.

(a) Microdrive (NLX-9) loaded with 8 tetrodes (arrow). (b) Example of a tetrode track (arrow) on a cresyl violet-stained coronal section of the striatum. We did not systematically perform electrolytic lesions. (c) Schematic localization of the tip of all tetrodes (n = 22, 5 rats) used in this study. (d) Quality of isolation of single-units recorded with tetrodes. Upper left: projection of two first principal components extracted from the spikes waveforms recorded from a single tetrode. 3 well-isolated clusters are clearly visible. Lower left: spikes from isolated units in the wide-band striatal LFP. Upper right: filtered spike waveforms. Lower right: auto- and cross-correlograms (± 30 ms) computed from the spike trains of the 3 units.

Supplementary Figure 4 Light on-responsive units.

Each row corresponds to an example unit recorded in a different animal. (a) Spike rasters (top) and average firing rate (bottom) aligned to treadmill onset. Red lines indicate light on, green lines treadmill onset, blue lines goal time. (b) Spike rasters (top) and average firing rate (bottom) aligned relative to the end of each running sequence, when animals crossed for the fist time the photodetector (black line). (c) Auto-correlograms of spiking activity during the entire recording session. (d) Spike rasters aligned relative to the time at which the left forelimb is stretched forward (red line, see Supplementary Fig. 2).

Supplementary Figure 5 Light off-responsive units.

Same panel structure than Supplementary Figure 4, but for units showing strong firing rate increases at the end of correct trials. Note that we make no assumption on the meaning of these modulations (reward consumption, sensorimotor activity associated with drinking-related movements,...). The trials in which there is no apparent change in firing rate correspond to incorrect trials.

Supplementary Figure 6 Forelimb-responsive units.

Same panel structure than Supplementary Figures 4 and 5 but for units that show strong rhythmical modulation of their firing rate coordinated with the oscillatory dynamics of forelimb movements during treadmill locomotion.

Supplementary Figure 7 Trial-by-trial correlations between firing rate and speed or position.

(a) Trial-by-trial color-coded instantaneous running speeds (top) and firing rates (bottom) for the unit shown in Figure 4b. Trials were sorted according to the time of maximum running speed. (b) Two error trials taken from a. Animal's position (top), running speed (middle) and firing rate of the unit (bottom). (c) Trial-by-trial color-coded positions (top) and instantaneous firing rates (bottom) during a single session for the unit shown in Figure 4d. Trials were sorted according to the time of termination of the front-rear-front sequence. (d), Two errors trials taken from c. Position (top) and firing rate of the unit (bottom) are shown. In the illustrative trials, the animal entered too early in the stop area. The gray area shows the portion of the trial corresponding to the performance of an archetypical front-rear-front sequence.

Supplementary Figure 8 Correlation between firing rate and running speed cannot be explained by gait transition.

(a) Scatter plot showing average firing rate versus speed (mean ± s.d.) for a speed-modulated unit during a session in which the animal galloped in a few trials. The partial correlation coefficient between running speed and firing rate was computed either with or without including galloping state (1 animal is galloping, 0 animal is not galloping) in our correlation matrix. (b) Example trials where speed (blue) and firing rate (black) are compared. To facilitate visual inspection, speed and firing rate were normalized to their maximum values in each trial. Frames in which the animal galloped are marked with yellow dots on top of the plot.

Supplementary Figure 9 The DLS of well-trained animals multiplex time, position, speed and acceleration.

(a) Median of the multiple correlation coefficients (all units) computed with regression models taking into account a single predictive variable (same color code than Fig. 4a), two (pink), three (orange) or four predictive variables (statistical differences against the model with 4 predictors are indicated as follows: * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001). (b) Cumulative distributions of the multiple correlation coefficients computed with regression models taking into account a single predictive variable and 4 predictive variables. (c) Median of Akaike information criterion (AIC) values computed for all recorded units, using the same regression models than in a Red dotted line indicates the AIC median value of the best model. (d) Differences in AIC values between the best model (4 predictors) and all the other models. (e) Statistical comparisons of the differences shown in d. Statistical differences in a and e were determined with Kruskall-Wallis and Tukey's HSD test.

Supplementary Figure 10 Predominance of linear relations between firing rate and kinematics variables in the DLS of well-trained animals.

(a,b) Mean firing rate vs. normalized position (blue), speed (gray) and acceleration (black) for two representative cells. Doted red lines indicate shuffled-derived global acceptance limits. (c) Distributions, for all the units analyzed, of the parts of the tuning curves that crossed the significance limits (red dots in a and b). (d) Fraction of units modulated by position, speed and acceleration. e, Correlation coefficients between firing rate and kinematics variables for all modulated units (median ± first and third quartiles). (f) Fraction of modulated units that display significant linear relationship between firing rate and the kinematics variables. (g,h) same cells than a and b. Surrogate tuning curves were pseudo-randomly constructed to test, for instance, (g, left panel, gray dashed lines) how much the interaction between firing rate and position was expected from the relation of these two variables with speed. The significant relation between acceleration and firing rate (g, right panel, black line) is entirely explained by the relationship of these two variables with position (blue). (i–l) Same than cf after correcting for the relations between the different variables.

Supplementary Figure 11 Temporal profile and magnitude of the firing rate modulations of striatal units recorded during hand-guided execution of the running sequence.

(a) Averaged modulation of the firing rate of all recorded units during hand-guided execution of the task, for the three naive animals (similar than Fig. 3). (b) Distribution of the maximum Z-scores for all the units recorded in each animal (left) or grouped per task condition (hand-guided versus well- trained, right). The maximum values of the Z-scores are slightly higher in the hand-guided group compared to the well-trained animals (Wilcoxon rank-sum test).

Supplementary Figure 12 Reduced correlations between firing rate and task variables cannot be explained by differences in running speed.

(a) Distribution (mean ± std) of running speeds for all the animals in which we computed correlations between firing rate and task variables. Hand-guided animals spent more time running at the treadmill speed (35–40 cm s−1) and less time running between 50 and 70 cm s−1 than well-trained animals. (b–d) In Rat 17, we recorded neuronal activity during hand-guided sessions and after training. Data were randomly removed (rectangles in a) to match the speed distribution in the two conditions. Speed distribution for Rat 17 in the hand-guided condition before (red) and after (gray) removing data (b). Speed distribution for Rat 17 after extensive training, before (blue) and after (gray) removing data (c). After correction, the speed distributions for Rat 17 are identical in both conditions (d). (e) Speed corrections did not affect the distribution of multiple correlation coefficients between firing rate and task variables (red versus dark gray and blue versus light gray). When speed distributions were matched between hand-guided and well-trained conditions the multiple correlation coefficients were highly increased after training (dark versus light gray, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, P < 0.05).

Supplementary Figure 13 Effect of bilateral injections of muscimol in the DLS on treadmill-based locomotion in naive animals.

(a,b) Trajectories during single sessions performed 10 min after bilateral injections (1 μl each side) of saline (a, black) or of 50 ng of muscimol (b, blue). During behavioral testing the animal ran freely during 20 s long trials with the treadmill speed set at 35 cm s−1. The two injections/behavioral tests were separated by 30 min (saline injection was performed first). (c) Running speed distributions were similar after saline and 50 ng muscimol injection. (d) Red line shows the difference between speed distributions. Dashed lines indicate the 5 % global confidence interval determined using a shuffling bootstrap procedure. (e,f) Similar than c,d but for the position of the animal on the treadmill. No significant differences were found. gl, similar than af but for injections of 500 ng of muscimol. The animal spent more time at low speeds and spend less time in the front of the treadmill after muscimol injection.

Supplementary Figure 14 Spread of muscimol injections, histology and behavioral effects.

(a), Injection cannula tracks and sites of injection in three rats. (b) Visualization of the spread of diffusion using a fluorescent dye. (c) Distributions of running speeds from a well-trained animal before and after muscimol injections. (d) Red line shows the difference between speed distributions before and after muscimol. Dashed lines indicate the 5 % global confidence interval determined using a shuffling/bootstrap procedure.

Supplementary Figure 15 Effect of bilateral injections of muscimol in the DLS of well-trained animals on running trajectories.

(a) Group effect of muscimol injections on percentage of correct trials. (b,c) Trajectories in trials that started similarly (red rectangle), one day before (a) and 10 min after (b) muscimol injection. Note the increased variability in entrance times (red dots) after muscimol injection. Trajectories were selected from the muscimol and pre-muscimol sessions shown in Figure 7a. (df) For each muscimol injection experiment, average trajectories (thick lines) and standard deviations (thin lines) in the session before (black), during (blue) and after (gray) muscimol injections. Experiments performed with goal times set at 4, 5 and 7 s are presented in columns d, e, and f respectively.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–15 (PDF 5621 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist (PDF 430 kb)

3 consecutive trials performed by Rat 9 during the first training session.

The bottom plot depicts the real time change in position of the rat while running on the treadmill. Trials 2 and 3 are cut at 8 s. The upper plot displays the complete trajectories for the 3 trials. Notice that the animal spent most of the trial in the front of the treadmill. (MP4 1378 kb)

7 consecutive trials performed by Rat 1 after 100 sessions of training.

The bottom plot depicts the positions of the fluorescent marker on the front left limb. The upper plot displays the smoothed trajectories (animal's position). Notice how regular is the running routine of the animal across trials. Last trial is incorrect. (MP4 2624 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rueda-Orozco, P., Robbe, D. The striatum multiplexes contextual and kinematic information to constrain motor habits execution. Nat Neurosci 18, 453–460 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3924

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3924

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing